[This
particular essay is pretty long so I’m going to put the moral of the story at
the beginning. Here I argue that we find it important to define attractiveness because
it is—naturally—the greatest indicator of the ability to bear physically
healthy children.]
The dynamics of a nightclub are always entertaining to me. I
normally try to stay away from true clubs (as in electronic music playing so
loud that ones’ thoughts are lost in the 808s—not to mention what the alcohol
does to peoples’ thoughts) but recently I was talked into going. As always, my
friend and I were discussing if he would talk to a particular girl in
attendance that night. As always, he was quick to dismiss anyone that I pointed
out. This made me think; how do we define facial attractiveness? Half of the
girls that I pointed out were decent—at least facially—in my opinion. Maybe I’m
just not that picky. But to my friend, many were not even worth consideration. Aside from the question of how we define attractiveness, why do we even find it
important to define attractiveness?
I was
curious to find out some of the answers that would be proposed for the
questions that I put forth so I asked my mother. She told me (while drooling over
the phone) that she defines attractiveness as a 6’5”, dark man with a bald head
and a muscular build. Not very helpful. I then went on to ask a different
female friend. She admitted that she did not actually know why she considered
some people attractive and others not so much.
The theory
of sexual selection may serve as the underlying theory as to why not just
humans, but rather many species find it important to define beauty.
Ronald Fisher, who set out to explain natural selection is his book
titled The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection stated that “ the
existence of sexual preference is to be ascribed to sexual selection are 1) the
existence of sexual preference at least in one sex and 2) bionomic conditions
in which such preference shall confer a reproductive advantage” Simply put,
there must be a reason for one sex to be choosy which will in turn allow for
the greatest number of offspring which will in turn survive to breed even more
offspring. Thus, species find it important to define attractiveness, in
its most elementary form, because doing so will allow them to mate with those
which will give the offspring the opportunity to survive to reproducing age. In
other words, attractive individuals appear to bear more fit genes.
Grammer
and Thornhill (1994) conducted a study to infer the relationship between facial
attractiveness and sexual selection in humans. They refer to sexual selection
according to the parasite theory, which states that individuals are likely to
choose mates based on characteristics that confer resistance to parasites: “The
parasite theory proposes that beauty of bodily form is perceived as a cue to
high parasite resistance by animals in choosing mates.” Thus, the offspring of
the chosen individuals will have the greatest fitness. Studies prior to
the one conducted by Grammy and Thornhill concluded that secondary sexual
traits are positively correlated with the quality of the immune system.
This is especially true in human males. As noted, “only healthy organisms
can afford the high testosterone handicap on the immune system that is
necessary for the production of elaborate sexual traits“ (as qtd. by Grammer
& Thornhill). High levels of testosterone at puberty influences
secondary sex characteristics such as the facial features of enlarged jaws,
chins and cheekbones. This caused Grammer and Thornhill to hypothesize that
such features would be considered attractive by women.
On the
other hand, heterozygosity (possessing one dominant and one recessive version
on a specific gene) also confers immune function; this correlates positively
with parasite resistance. Individuals heterozygous for MHC genes display
greater ability to mount an immune response to a wider range of antigens.
Contrary to secondary sex traits, which increase in intensity as sex hormones
increase during puberty, heterozygosity results with average trait expression.
In other words, heterozygosity correlates positively with average trait
expression. Therefore the scientists hypothesized that average facial
traits in women would be considered attractive by males.
To test
the hypothesis, the scientists allowed participants to rank both computer
generated faces in order to create faces with average facial dimensions and
structures and faces with extreme facial dimensions and structures. As
hypothesized, women found the males to be attractive when the features were
extreme and males found the women to be attractive when the features were
average. These findings imply that, though one may find it difficult to
define what an attractive face looks like, they somehow have the ability to
define an attractive face that simultaneously corresponds to a healthy face
that will give rise to healthy children.
I chose
to begin my discussion concerning sexual selection with a human example because
it is naturally easier to relate to humans. I must concede, however, and admit
that some problems arise when conducting studies such as these in humans. The
first one that comes to my mind is the influence of modern media in humans’
evaluation of attractiveness of individuals. Also, there are other
characteristics, which may influence humans’ discretion. There is nothing
inherently healthy-seeming about my mom’s preference in dark, 6’5 black men
with an athletic build (possibly it’s the fact that this type of man may be
able to rescue her from a burning house? My point is that there are a million
ad hoc explanations that one could generate for any physical characteristic).
Nevertheless, due many external influences, my mom finds this class of males
attractive. Fortunately, there are many other examples of sexual selection in
nature. One such example is the African long-tailed widow bird (which I will
refer to as simply widows), as discussed by Richard Dawkins in The Blind
Watchmaker.
In
widows, females have the luxury of choosing which males they will mate with.
Males that are able to attract mates are usually very successful to the point
where one male may attract multiple mates. As explained by Dawkins: “This means
that here is a surplus of males in one population who do not reproduce. This,
in turn, means that females have no difficulty in finding mates, and are in a
position to be choosy.” What gives the male birds the ability females?
The answer lies in their exaggerated tail feathers. Male widows with longer
tails more often then not win the hearts of female widows (the length of the
tail has a functional limit due to the fact that it is burdensome to carry and
makes the males prone to predation; I elect not discuss the relationship of
sexual selection and natural selection in this essay).
This
leads us to an important question, why are long tails preferred? Dawkins
explains that the answer lies within the widow genome: “Female preference is a
quantitative variable, and we can assume that it is under the control of
polygenes in just the same kind of way as male tail length itself.” It is
important to note that the genes for male preference, as well as the genes for
tail length, are not sexist. That is to say that they do not discriminate based
on sex— although extraordinary tail length may only be expressed in males, the
genes remain in women as well. Likewise, preference is not undertaken in males
but still exists. This is an essential to our understanding of sexual selection
because we now are able to establish the fact that tail length, as well as
preference for tail length, is something that is passed on from generation to
generation together. Dawkins explains that the preference for long tails
specifically could have arisen by chance: “the moment a majority, however
slight, started to accrue among females for one type of preference rather than
the other, that majority would be reinforced in subsequent generations.” In
this way, since preference and tail length are inherited together, there will
be a population wide preference for long tails that will
only intensify over time (once again, I must note that there exists a
functional limit).
As we
have seen, sexual selection may allow one specific sex to infer which mates are
most suitable for rearing fit offspring. In considering the benefits that
species may receive in carefully choosing a mate based off of physical
characteristics, I was lead to investigate why this type of behavior is not
employed by more species. I came across M. Bastocks’ and a study in 1956 conducted on Drosophila melanogaster or fruit flies.
In order to study sexual selection, Bastock
mutated genes that determine the color of the body of the flies. He then
observed the mutated flies behavior and ability to attract a mate. In an
earlier study conducted by C. Petit in 1954, flies were randomly mutated via
x-ray treatment and also observed the flies’ sexual activity. Together, they
observed that the flies with mutated body color tried much harder to attract a
mate and were often unsuccessful in their attempts. On the other hand, flies
lacking genes vital to their physiology did not have trouble attracting a mate.
However, overtime these flies were selected against due to pleiotropic effects
caused by the mutations—phenotypes that arose via indirect interaction of the
gene in question were unfavorably looked upon.
Upon
further reflection, these results actually make a lot of sense. Hypothetically
speaking, let’s say I spotted a woman with a blue face at my favorite bar. Call
me crazy, but my first reaction wouldn’t be, “Wow, she would be the perfect
mother for my children!” Rather, my response would most likely be along the
lines of, “RUN!!!” Or, at the very least, she would have a lot of explaining to
do before I consider even giving her my number, not to mention the possibility
of a relationship. On the other hand, let’s say I spotted a woman that looked
like Halle Berry’s sister at a bar. Unfortunately, she has sickle cell anemia.
My first reaction wouldn’t be, “I feel like she has a very serious
physiological ailment.” My first reaction would probably be, “get her phone
number!” But, after generations of
offspring with sickle cell anemia, traits that may be associated with sickle
cell anemia will soon be looked at with caution; my friends’ children would
know to not touch a woman that looks like Halle Berry for fear of
sickle cell anemia.
I admit
that my example is an exaggeration. In principle, however, the same phenomenon
is occurring in the studied flies. Although they may not be selecting mates
based on a particular physical trait, they have an ability to select against
traits that are abnormal, which have a higher probability of birthing unfit
offspring—sexual selection!
But why
is sexual selection so profound in some species and not others? Consider fruit
flies for a moment; it is possible that a couple of the body color mutants (by
chance) attract a mate and (by chance) give rise to offspring with increased
fitness. In other words, these random combinations happen to bear super fruit
flies! Not only would the majority of flies within the population tend to shift
in color due to their increased fitness, but women preference would also shift
away from the standard color of gray to the new color, as discussed earlier. As
Bastock put it, “Although [the mutation’s] effect is deleterious to mating
success nevertheless it could be incidentally incorporated in an isolated
population if the mutant concerned were useful in any other sense” (as qtd. By
Claudine Petit and Lee Ehrman) Thus, although many species may not have
elaborate advertisements in their quest to attract a mate, one could argue that
sexual selection is a theory that can be applied to all species that have two
separate and distinct sexes.
As I have
shown, the effects of sexual selection can play a large role in the evolution
of many species where there are two distinct species and at least one has the
luxury of choosing a mate. Even more, the sexual selection that occurs my have
important consequences on the success of future generations. In species where
there is no need for one sex to be choosy, specific behaviors and
characteristics may not need to evolve, thus a specific display of sexual
selection may not be observed. According to Grammer and Thornhill, humans are
an example of a species that employs sexual selection.
The next
time your friend tells you that you worry too much about the physical
appearance of your significant other, blame evolution.